版權(quán)說明:本文檔由用戶提供并上傳,收益歸屬內(nèi)容提供方,若內(nèi)容存在侵權(quán),請進行舉報或認領
文檔簡介
1、<p><b> 外文文獻</b></p><p> What's Loyalty? </p><p> Michael J. Withey 1 and William H. Cooper </p><p> Loyalty in organizations has proved difficult to predi
2、ct. One reason is that loyalty is complex and poorly understood. We report two studies that attempt to understand and predict loyalty by focusing on two components of the construct: active-constructive loyalty and passiv
3、e-constructive loyalty. In the first study, we found that active acts of loyalty were predicted by variables quite different from those that predicted passive loyalty. The second study found that people identified by pee
4、rs as </p><p> KEY WORDS: loyalty ; commitment; active; passive.</p><p> INTRODUCTION </p><p> What is loyalty? In this article, we will suggest some ways to consider this qu
5、estion. Our starting point is Hirschman's (1970) treatment of exit, voice, and loyalty. Hirschman offers exit and voice as distinct responses when firms, organizations, and states are facing decline. What Hirschman m
6、eans by loyalty is less clear. He first refers to loyalty as a form of attachment that makes voice more attractive when exit is available (Hirschman, 1970, p. 77). He describes loyalty as the product of (p</p><
7、;p> Other treatments of loyalty have also been varied. They include, for example, Rusbult, Farrell, Rogers, and Mainous's (1988) characterization of loyalty as pas- sive-constructive behavior (e.g., being quietly
8、 supportive and being patient); Kolar- ska and Aldrich's (1980) work, which, referring to the response, "silence" rather than loyalty; and Graham's (1990) discussion of loyalty as an attitude without an
9、y behavioral component. </p><p> These depictions of loyalty mirror the ambiguity of the construct in ordinary language. We sometimes speak of loyalty as an attitude, other times of loyalty as behavior. Wit
10、hin the loyalty behavior domain, there are both active elements (doing things that are supportive of someone or something) and passive elements (being quiet while exhibiting patient forbearance). In the present research,
11、 we will consider loyalty as a behavior and discuss the attitudinal elements of loyalty in terms of organiz</p><p> Summary of the Loyalty Literature </p><p> Recently, several studies have co
12、nsidered Hirschman's loyalty construct. Far- reU (1983) classified exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect with the use of multidimensional scaling techniques. This study measured loyalty in passive terms, such as "qu
13、ietly doing my job and letting higher-ups make the decisions," and found that loyalty is indeed conceptually separable from the other responses. Loyalty, however, did not conform exactly to expectations, being shown
14、 to be passive (as expected) but slightly</p><p> nature of the loyalty construct and how to measure it.</p><p> Loyalty has been investigated in two recent studies of exit, voice and loyalty.
15、 In the first, Rusbult et al. (1988) found support for investment model predictions of loyalty. Specifically, loyalty was more likely to occur under conditions of high prior satisfaction, high investments, and relatively
16、 few alternatives. This view of loy- alty is consistent with a passive, constructive construct. In the second study, Withey and Cooper (1989) found quite different results. Loyalty was associated with </p><p&g
17、t; The measurement problem has been described by Cooper, Dyke, and Kay (1990) in terms of construct validity: loyalty has been operationalized in ways that do not match the loyalty construct. While loyalty is defined as
18、 supporting the or-ganization, items used to measure loyalty are too narrow and do not conform to most people's notion of support. The Cooper, Dyke, and Kay study used the act frequency methodology (Buss & Craik,
19、 1983) to assess the prototypicality of a num- </p><p> ber of acts of loyalty, including the acts used by Farrell (1983) to measure loyalty. Interestingly, the three items used in both the Farrell and the
20、Withey and Cooper studies cited above (e.g., "say nothing to others and assume things will work out") ranked 99th, 101st, and 102nd among 103 acts of loyalty. More prototypical were acts such as "give some
21、thing extra when the organization needs it" (lst) and "do things above and beyond the call without being asked" (4th). </p><p> The Cooper et al. study supports the claim that there is a cons
22、truct validity problem and suggests that previous research on loyalty has left much of the domain of loyalty unassessed. One promising way to approach this problem is to make a distinction between active and passive loya
23、lty. This distinction was first raised by Farrell (1983) in categorizing the exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect responses, but it may also prove useful in interpreting loyalty. The distinction between active and passiv<
24、;/p><p> In summary, more attention is needed at both the conceptual and the mea-surement level. On the conceptual level, loyalty needs to be investigated as both active and passive (and possibly as both const
25、ructive and destructive, again following Farrell's categorization). On the measurement level, distinct measures of the dif- ferent forms of loyalty are necessary. The present research is designed to begin to answer t
26、hese needs.</p><p> Goals of the Present Research </p><p> To begin the investigation of active and passive loyalty, the present research poses the following general questions. First, can diff
27、erential predictors of active- constructive and passive-constructive loyalty be identified? Second, what is the re- lationship between active and passive loyalty and to what extent are they related over time? Third, in w
28、hat ways are the actions of people who are defined as loyal different from actions of people who are defined as not being loyal? Two studies were c</p><p><b> STUDY 1 </b></p><p>
29、The first study is a cross-sectional study which measures the different forms of loyalty and a set of independent variables that are thought to predict loyalty. Because Study 1 is a follow-up study on the sample reported
30、 by Withey and Cooper (1989), it is possible to assess relationships among active and passive loyalty over a six-year time lapse. It is also possible to conduct a six-year longitudinal study of the predictors of active a
31、nd passive loyalty. </p><p> Methodology </p><p> Data were collected during the summer of 1990 through a survey mailed to those graduates of the Queen's University Bachelor of Commerce pr
32、ogram who had, in 1984, participated in the study reported by Withey and Cooper (1989). Of the 360 potential respondents, 210 returned their questionnaires representing a re- sponse rate of 58.0%. Sixty-eight percent of
33、the respondents were male, all had undergraduate degrees, their average age was approximately 33 and their average work experience was approxi</p><p> Descriptive statistics for all measures appear in Table
34、 I. All, excepting locus of control which uses a 23-item forced choice format, are 5-point Likert scales. The independent variables were measured in terms of agreement with the statement; the loyalty measures were based
35、on frequency of engaging in the behavior. With the exceptions noted below, all scales achieved a satisfactory level of interitem re-liability. Low internal consistency would be expected to attenuate correlation coef- fic
36、ients</p><p> A total of nine independent variables was included. "Exit costs" include skill specificity (e.g., "My present job involves skills which would be useful in many other organizatio
37、ns," reversed) and sunk costs (e.g., "If I left this job, my pension plan losses would be significant."), which are both aspects of Becker's (1960) side bets, and investments (Rusbult et aL, 1988). The
38、 sunk costs scale had low internal con-sistency. "Voice costs" asked about the effort required to bring about change and t</p><p> Analysis was conducted by using correlations and regression analy
39、sis to assess the ability of the independent variables to predict each form of loyalty. Further, correlations between independent variables and loyalty measured in 1984 and forms of loyalty measured in 1990 are reported.
40、 Several independent variables were associated with passive loyalty (see Table II). The passive loyalist emerged as being dissatisfied and uncommitted, having a relatively external locus of control, and facing high </
41、p><p> Finally, the zero-order correlation between the two forms of loyalty is negative and significant .Because the present study is a follow-up of an earlier study, it is possible to present six-year longitu
42、dinal results. All measures described above (except active loyalty) were measured in the earlier study. In spite of the long time lag, a strong positive correlation was found between passive loyalty in 1984 and in 1990 ,
43、 and a negative correlation was found between passive loyalty in 1984 and acti</p><p> Further, some modest correlations were found between the independent vari-ables measured in 1984 and both forms of loya
44、lty measured in 1990. Specifically, passive loyalty was negatively related to belief in the possibility of improvement, organizational commitment , and locus of control , and positively related to skill specificity . Act
45、ive loyalty was related only to skill specificity . </p><p> Study 1 provides clear evidence for two distinct types of loyalty. First, the findings for passive loyalty that Withey and Cooper (1989) reported
46、 as counterin- tuitive were replicated. The earlier study had expected to find loyalty as a passive but essentially supportive behavior; instead loyalty emerged as something that re-sembled entrapment rather than support
47、. Loyalty as entrapment was found in the present study as well, but a more constructive and active form was identified as an </p><p> alternative behavior. The contribution of Study 1, then, is to distingui
48、sh between two types of loyalty and to demonstrate that each is associated with different in- dividual characteristics. These findings appear more robust when it is realized that at least some of them hold over a six-yea
49、r time lag. </p><p> The results suggest future directions for research. In particular, it becomes necessary to relate the different forms of loyalty to other behavioral constructs within the exit, voice, a
50、nd loyalty framework. While passive loyalty resembles ne-glect, active loyalty resembles voice. Such relationships need clarification in future research.</p><p> In addition, the loyalty typology could be b
51、roadened to investigate constructive and destructive forms of loyalty as well. Given that clear differences have been found between active and passive loyalty, there is reason to expect that loyalty is a multi-dimensiona
52、l construct. The potential exists to identify four distinct types of loyalty, each with its own unique characteristics. For example, the active constructive type of loyalty might resemble voice while passive constructive
53、 loyalty would b</p><p><b> STUDY 2 </b></p><p> Study 2 is designed to assess whether people who are perceived as loyal en-gage in different acts than do people who are perceived
54、as not being loyal. In essence, the study asks what it is that loyal people do that defines them as loyal. </p><p> To assess differences between employees with high and low loyalty, 200 questionnaires were
55、 mailed to recent graduates of Queen's University's School of Business. One hundred questionnaires asked respondents to think of a target person who, to them, typified high organizational loyalty (defined for res
56、pondents as pro- viding support for the organization, including active and passive support); the remaining 100 questionnaires asked respondents to think of a target person who, to them, typified low</p><p>
57、 Sixty-three per cent of the respondents were male. The average age of the respondents was 35.4 years and they had an average work experience of 11.8 years. The respondent had observed his or her target person for an ave
58、rage of 5.4 years. The average age and organizational tenure of the target persons were 39.6 and 10.0 years, respectively. Eighty-three percent of the target persons were male. </p><p> The questionnaire co
59、ntained, among other things, 105 behavioral acts. The acts included the 100 loyalty acts reported in Cooper et al.'s act frequency study (1990), Farrelrs (1983) three items, and two test acts designed to detect carel
60、ess respondents. </p><p> The analysis was designed to allow the identification of loyalty acts that dif- ferentiated between high and low loyalty. Comparisons were made to determine which items had signifi
61、cantly different scores for high- and low-loyalty target persons. All 105 acts were included in the analysis. Thus, the analysis will determine which active and/or passive acts of loyalty distinguish people who are loyal
62、 from those who are not. </p><p> The results indicate that a clear pattern was found regarding which of the 105 acts differentiated between high- and low-loyalty target persons. In particular, the acts tha
63、t were best able to differentiate were those that were in the top quartile of the Cooper et al. (1990) prototypicality study, that is, the most prototypical loyalty acts. Of the 10 most prototypical acts, all are clearly
64、 active in nature (and three comprised the active loyalty measure in Study 1) and all differentiated between</p><p> None of the three acts used to measure passive loyalty in Study 1 (those adapted from Far
65、rell, 1983) nor the two test acts were able to differentiate people described as loyal from those described as not loyal. The failure of the test acts to differentiate lends credibility to the methodology employed in Stu
66、dy 2; the failure of the passive loyalty acts to differentiate will be discussed below. </p><p> The results of Study 2 must be considered in only a tentative way because of the small sample size. Nonethele
67、ss, the clear pattern of the results, given the small sample, lends them credibility. The results of Study 2 are consistent with Buss and Craik's (1983) idea of validity gradients. For Buss and Craik, the centrality
68、of acts for a domain is indexed by the act's prototypicality: the higher the prototypicality, the more central the act is to the domain. When acts are central, they are more r</p><p> An implication is
69、that central acts will have a higher construct validity than will acts at the periphery. This is precisely what we found. The top 25 acts all differed in the predicted direction for high- and low-loyalty individuals. The
70、 middle quartiles differed less, and the acts in the bottom quartile were the least successful. In addition, the Farrell acts (which ranked very low in prototypicality) did not differentiate between high and low loyalist
71、s. </p><p> Thus, the more central the acts were, the more likely they were to be per- formed more by high loyalists than low loyalists. This was not true for the peripheral acts of loyalty, including the t
72、hree Farrell acts. These results are as predicted by Buss and Craik's validity gradient concept. They point to the usefulness of the act frequency method as a basis for developing measures of organizational behaviors
73、, including loyalty. </p><p> The results of Study 2 increase our confidence in both the measures of loyalty used in Study 1 and our ability to predict the different forms of loyalty. Further, the distincti
74、on between active and passive loyalty made in Study 1 ig supported. Finally, because the most prototypical acts are much more active than the least prototypical acts, the conclusion can be drawn that it is active and not
75、 passive loyalty that is used by observers to determine loyalty in co-workers. </p><p> The two studies reported in this article begin to distinguish between active and passive forms of loyalty. In Study 1,
76、 it was shown that active loyalty and passive loyalty are associated with different individual characteristics. In Study 2, it was shown that only the loyalty items that are central to the loyalty construct, that is, loy
77、alty acts that are active, are able to distinguish loyal people from people who are not loyal. This distinction is an important step in the development of our un-</p><p><b> 什么是忠誠?</b></p>
78、<p> 邁克爾·j . Withey 1和威廉·h·庫珀</p><p> 忠誠于組織已被證明是難以預測的。原因之一是,忠誠是復雜和難以理解。我們報告兩項研究,試圖理解和預測忠誠著眼于柔性的兩部分。在最初的研究中,我們發(fā)現(xiàn)積極行為的預測變量的忠誠度,預測不同被動的忠誠。第二項研究發(fā)現(xiàn)人們識別的同學,員工進行更多的積極套忠誠比那些被確認為low-loyalty員工。
79、我們認為雙方組成的忠誠。</p><p> 關(guān)鍵詞: 忠誠; 承諾; 主動; 被動</p><p><b> 介紹</b></p><p> 什么是忠誠?在這篇文章中,我們將提出一些方法來考慮這個問題。我們的出發(fā)點是Hirschman的(1970)治療退出,聲音,和忠誠度。Hirschman提供出口和聲音是截然不同的反應,企業(yè)、組
80、織和國家正面臨著衰退。什么Hirschman意味著忠誠的關(guān)系尚不清楚。他指的是忠誠的,作為一種連線,使聲音時,出口更具吸引力,1970年,Hirschman(p。第77條)。他描述了忠誠的產(chǎn)物(主要是經(jīng)濟因素的個體,使出口昂貴的組織和降低的聲音。有時,Hirschman描述作為一種態(tài)度忠誠影響程度進行出口和聲音的使用。在其他時候他講的忠誠是作為一種行為的個體見sub -端口的組織。</p><p> 其他療法也
81、被不同的忠誠。他們包括,例如,Rusbult,法瑞爾,羅杰斯和Mainous的(1988)的行為,如不e忠誠(例如,支持和病人安靜;Kolar - ska(1980年),這里的工作,它的回應,"沉默”,而不是“忠誠、格雷厄姆(1990)討論的忠誠作為一種態(tài)度沒有任何動作組成。</p><p> 這些描寫的忠誠的鏡子里曖昧的構(gòu)建普通語言。我們有時會說話的忠誠度,有時作為一種態(tài)度忠誠的行為。在忠誠行為領域
82、,既有積極因素(在那些支持某人或某事)和被動元件(安靜同時展出的病人寬容)。在目前的研究中,我們會考慮忠誠為行為和討論態(tài)度的元素的組織承諾的忠誠,定義為共享價值觀的公司(波特.讓,1974年)。進一步,我們將努力使我們的理解忠誠的行為之間的區(qū)別主動和被動元件的忠誠。</p><p> 最近幾項研究已經(jīng)被認為是Hirschman的忠誠。目前拉(1983)的分類,出口,聲音,忠誠,而忽視了用多維尺度的技巧。本研究中
83、測量的忠誠度,諸如“被動靜靜地做我的工作,并讓上級做出的決定,”和發(fā)現(xiàn)的忠誠是確實從其他反應。離忠誠,但并不完全符合預期時,被證明是被動的(如預期的那樣),但是稍微破壞性的組織(不是有建設性的,如預期的那樣)。這個意想不到的情形,sification cla在后續(xù)的研究,忠誠的真正問題提出了質(zhì)疑大自然的構(gòu)建和如何衡量它。</p><p> 忠誠已被調(diào)查在最近的兩項研究的聲音和忠誠度。出口,首先,Rusbult等
84、。(1988)發(fā)現(xiàn)支持投資模型的預測的忠誠。確切地說,忠誠是更容易發(fā)生前的條件下,高滿意度較高,相對較少的投資選擇。這種觀點是一致的,alty何來是被動的,也有建設性的建構(gòu)。在第二項研究,Withey和庫珀(1989)發(fā)現(xiàn)截然不同的結(jié)果。忠誠與滿意度、低前低投資、低組織承諾,外部內(nèi)外控,信仰的改善情況不太可能。沒有重新關(guān)系與選擇。因此,忠實呈現(xiàn)出一種清晰破壞性的外觀。這些結(jié)果導致Withey和庫珀看起來都一樣的測量和觀念化忠誠的解釋。&
85、lt;/p><p> 庫珀等研究支持這個聲稱有一個建構(gòu)效度的問題和建議以前的研究在忠誠的還有很多領域的忠誠。一個有前途的辦法來解決這個問題是對這兩方面加以區(qū)分:主動和被動的忠誠。這是首次提出的法瑞爾(1983)出境,聲音分類,忠誠,而忽視了反應,但是它也可以用于解釋的忠誠。主動與被動的區(qū)別也明顯的物品時,庫柏研究等。(1990)。那是一個關(guān)鍵的區(qū)別,法瑞爾項目和忠誠地忠誠度項目就是前者是活躍的,而后者是消極的。&l
86、t;/p><p> 綜上所述,更注重的是需要在概念和測量水平。在這個概念的層次,忠誠需要研究既是主動和被動(可能是破壞性的,又都有建設性的,下面的法瑞爾的分類)。在測量水平,不同的措施進行不同形式的忠誠度,是必要的。本研究旨在回答這些需求。</p><p> 開始調(diào)查的主動和被動的忠誠,本研究提出下列問題。首先,可微分的預測,活躍——建設性忠誠確認嗎?第二,什么是再保險——主動和被動忠誠度
87、之間的關(guān)系到什么程度就相關(guān)?第三,在哪些方面是行動的人定義為不同的行為忠誠的人被定義為不忠誠?兩項研究來解決這些問題。</p><p><b> 研究1</b></p><p> 第一個研究是一個橫斷面研究方法的不同形式的忠誠和一套獨立變量,它被認為是預測的忠誠。由于書房是后續(xù)研究樣本W(wǎng)ithey報告和庫珀(1989),它是可能的,來評估主動和被動忠誠度之間的關(guān)系
88、,并在六年的時間間隔。它也可以進行了六年的縱向研究的主動和被動的忠誠。</p><p> “信仰”的可能性,提高測量的措施9-item稱為雜合-國家的迅速接受新事物的能力去改變(如,“這辦公室組織如此接受員工的輸入”)和Graen,Liden,Hoers(1982)的領導成員關(guān)系/?!肮ぷ鳚M意”是用Brayfield測量及大型的(1951年),18-item規(guī)模。“內(nèi)外控”,應用Rotter(1966年)的標準
89、23-item強迫選擇工具?!傲俊钡慕M織承諾用波特等的(1974年)??蛇x擇的“工作”是用兩項衡量價格和Bluedorn(1979年)的規(guī)模,指的是比目前的工作?!蓚€從屬變量都包括在內(nèi)?!氨粍拥闹艺\”是測量法瑞爾的(1983)3-item測量、適應指忠實行為的發(fā)生率工作背景。這3項組成的規(guī)模是“耐心地等待著,希望能解決任何問題,”“悄悄地在做自己的工作,讓上級做出的決定,”和“什么也沒有說到別人,假設的事情就會順利了?!本拖裰疤岬降?/p>
90、,這些物品都有很低的典型性評級。“積極的忠誠”是用測3-item規(guī)模等?;趲扃甑男袨檠芯俊nl率基于篩選項目和缺乏概念中退出,聲音,而忽視了。這3項組成的規(guī)模是:“給一些額外的組織需要時,“把公司的信息嚴格保密,”和“做事情超出了電話。</p><p> 忠誠度項目提交因素分析。雙結(jié)構(gòu)清晰浮現(xiàn)。第一個因素包括三個被動的忠誠度項目(因子載荷五花八門,從0.71,0.79);第二個因素包括三個忠誠度項目(因子載荷五
91、花八門,從0.61,0.77)。交叉因子載荷0.20或以下。這個因素分析論證,忠誠是截然不同的。然而,兩種方式的忠誠有相對較低的內(nèi)在的一致性。進一步了解所有,但最后的測量,可以發(fā)現(xiàn),在Withey和庫珀(1989年);進一步了解過去的測量,可以發(fā)現(xiàn),在庫柏等。(1990)。通過相關(guān)性分析和回歸分析評估獨立變量預測的各種形式的忠誠。進一步的,獨立變量之間的相關(guān)關(guān)系,忠誠于1984年,測測忠誠于1990年被報道。幾個獨立變量是被動的忠誠(見
92、圖表)。作為被動的親信出現(xiàn)不滿和中立,有一個相對外部內(nèi)外控、面對高昂的成本的變化,似乎不可能。此結(jié)果組型復制的結(jié)果Withey和庫珀(1989)。</p><p> 結(jié)果發(fā)現(xiàn)不同的活躍的忠誠?;钴S的忠誠是具有高的承諾——ciated亢戰(zhàn)副教授赴和滿意,認為情況是有可能得以改善的成本,而不是被動的行動。確切地說,活躍的忠誠與出口成本(即高心理、投資),但是經(jīng)濟出口成本(例如,技能的特異性,沉沒成本)和低成本的聲音
93、。值得注意的是,大部分的獨立變量的跡象是不同的相關(guān)系數(shù)為主動和被動的忠誠。</p><p> 在回歸分析中,也有明顯區(qū)別的忠誠。為被動的忠誠、內(nèi)外控、組織承諾有顯著,而活躍的忠誠度貝慈,兩個出口成本與組織承諾是很重要的。值得注意的是,承諾進入兩者的回歸方程,但相反的效果,增加活躍的忠誠,但減少消極的忠誠。</p><p> 最后,零級的相關(guān)性兩種形式的忠誠是顯著的,因為目前的研究是一項
94、較早的研究中,隨訪是可能的結(jié)果呈現(xiàn)縱。所有上述措施(除了活躍的忠誠度)在較早的研究。盡管長時間的延遲,有很強的正相關(guān),忠誠于1984年與消極1990年呈負相關(guān),與被動的忠誠度之間在1984年和活躍的忠誠于1990年。不僅如此,一些適度的相關(guān)量之間的vari-ables獨立于1984年,兩種形式的忠誠度。1990年測量確切地說,是被動的忠誠信念的負相關(guān)的可能性、組織承諾及改善內(nèi)外控、正相關(guān)技術(shù)的特殊性?;钴S的忠誠是相關(guān)的技巧只特異性。&l
95、t;/p><p> 研究提供了明確的證據(jù),1兩種不同形式的忠誠。首先,這項發(fā)現(xiàn)為被動的忠誠,Withey和庫珀(1989)報道——克隆。早期的研究已經(jīng)試圖找到,但作為一種被動忠誠,忠誠的支持性行為而出現(xiàn)的東西,而不是支持誘捕。作為陷阱中發(fā)現(xiàn)了忠誠本研究,但更有建設性的、主動式被確認為一可供選擇的行為。研究的貢獻,是1,然后去區(qū)分兩種類型的忠誠和顯示每個與不同的特點。這些發(fā)現(xiàn)出現(xiàn)具有更強的魯棒性時,它是意識到其中的一
96、些持有超過六年的時間延遲?!?lt;/p><p> 結(jié)果表明,未來的研究方向。在特殊情況下,成為必要的不同形式的忠誠度與其他行為建立在出口處,聲音,和忠誠的框架。就像忠誠,而消極的聲音。就像活躍的忠誠這樣的關(guān)系需要澄清在未來的研究方向。</p><p> 此外,忠誠可以拓寬了類型學研究建設性和破壞性的忠誠。鑒于明顯的差異已經(jīng)發(fā)現(xiàn)主動和被動忠誠,有理由期待的忠誠是一個多維構(gòu)建。識別可能存在的
97、四種不同形式的忠誠度,每個人都有自己獨特的特點。例如,積極的建設性的類型的忠誠,而消極的聲音可能會像忠誠將相似建設性的想法Hirschman安靜的支持。被動的破壞性的忠誠度似乎就像疏忽,活躍的破壞性的忠誠,而難以構(gòu)思,可被視為違法行為,如執(zhí)行所建議的組織。</p><p><b> 研究2</b></p><p> 二是用來評估研究的患者是否在不同看作是忠誠的行為
98、,而不是做那些被視為不忠誠。在本質(zhì)上,書房要求是什么,人們做了他們的忠誠。</p><p> 評估員工之間的差異與高與低的忠誠度,200問卷寄到女大學剛畢業(yè)的學生,學校的業(yè)務。一百問卷要求受訪者認為目標的人,對于他們來說,顯示高的組織忠誠(定義為受訪者作為職業(yè)-瓦支持組織,包括主動和被動支護);其余100問卷調(diào)查想問一個目標的人,對于他們來說,顯示低組織的忠誠。的受訪者問如何頻繁的目標的人從事各種不同的行為。包
溫馨提示
- 1. 本站所有資源如無特殊說明,都需要本地電腦安裝OFFICE2007和PDF閱讀器。圖紙軟件為CAD,CAXA,PROE,UG,SolidWorks等.壓縮文件請下載最新的WinRAR軟件解壓。
- 2. 本站的文檔不包含任何第三方提供的附件圖紙等,如果需要附件,請聯(lián)系上傳者。文件的所有權(quán)益歸上傳用戶所有。
- 3. 本站RAR壓縮包中若帶圖紙,網(wǎng)頁內(nèi)容里面會有圖紙預覽,若沒有圖紙預覽就沒有圖紙。
- 4. 未經(jīng)權(quán)益所有人同意不得將文件中的內(nèi)容挪作商業(yè)或盈利用途。
- 5. 眾賞文庫僅提供信息存儲空間,僅對用戶上傳內(nèi)容的表現(xiàn)方式做保護處理,對用戶上傳分享的文檔內(nèi)容本身不做任何修改或編輯,并不能對任何下載內(nèi)容負責。
- 6. 下載文件中如有侵權(quán)或不適當內(nèi)容,請與我們聯(lián)系,我們立即糾正。
- 7. 本站不保證下載資源的準確性、安全性和完整性, 同時也不承擔用戶因使用這些下載資源對自己和他人造成任何形式的傷害或損失。
最新文檔
- 什么是信托?【外文翻譯】
- 什么是集群【外文翻譯】
- 什么是博客【外文翻譯】
- 外文翻譯----什么是數(shù)據(jù)挖掘
- 什么是庫存管理?【外文翻譯】
- 什么是營銷策略-外文翻譯
- [雙語翻譯]安全外文翻譯--什么是安全科學?
- 外文翻譯-----數(shù)據(jù)挖掘什么是數(shù)據(jù)挖掘?
- 什么是金融風險管理【外文翻譯】
- [雙語翻譯]安全外文翻譯--什么是安全科學?(英文)
- [雙語翻譯]安全外文翻譯--什么是安全科學中英全
- 2014年安全外文翻譯--什么是安全科學?
- 什么是翻譯評論
- 電子商務——什么是電子商務?【外文翻譯】
- 什么是液壓系統(tǒng)設計外文文獻翻譯.doc
- 什么是液壓系統(tǒng)設計外文文獻翻譯.doc
- 什么是液壓系統(tǒng)設計外文文獻翻譯.doc
- 什么是第三方物流【外文翻譯】
- 什么是液壓系統(tǒng)設計外文文獻翻譯.doc
- 什么是直銷?定義,觀點和研究議程【外文翻譯】
評論
0/150
提交評論